The everyday activities above require spatial ability. Spatial ability involves perceiving the location, dimension and properties of objects and their relationships to one another. Not only is spatial ability core to everyday living, but spatial ability in childhood predicts adult expertise in Science, Technology, Engineering and Mathematics (STEM) (Wai et al., 2009). This is unsurprising, given many examples of spatial skills that are integral to STEM professions (e.g., understanding graphs and electronic configurations).
Engineering, a key STEM industry, annually contributes 21.4% (£1.2 trillion) to the UK economy (Engineering UK, 2018), yet over £1.5bn in losses are reported per year due to STEM skills shortages (IET, 2021). Despite the importance of spatial thinking, educational policy in England has very limited focus on spatial thinking, particularly when compared to the importance placed on literacy skills.
Not only is spatial ability core to everyday living, but spatial ability in childhood predicts adult expertise in Science, Technology, Engineering and Mathematics.
One way to address school readiness is to look beyond subject knowledge and to place a higher value on spatial thinking skills, as measured by CAT4.
GL Assessment’s recent YouGov poll indicated that primary and secondary teachers are concerned about school readiness for students starting Y7 this year. One way to address school readiness is to look beyond subject knowledge and to place a higher value on spatial thinking skills, as measured by CAT4.
Spatial activities are effective in improving spatial thinking skills and are a means to improving academic attainment in STEM subjects (Uttal et al., 2013). For example, it has been consistently demonstrated that spatial training increases primary and secondary achievement in mathematics (Hawes et al., 2022). This is logical, given that understanding science and mathematics depends heavily on being able to read diagrams, rearrange formulae and interpret representations at different scales.
Similarly, mathematics requires an understanding of shape, symmetry and numerical relationships, all of which require spatial skills, whilst the core problem solving and interpretation skills that are drawn upon in STEM subjects require visualisation, a key spatial skill.
There are measures and strategies that practitioners can adopt to nurture spatial thinking. For children with high spatial ability (see Samira’s story) these strategies suit their learning profile. However, because spatial thinking is malleable, nurturing spatial thinking is beneficial for the full range of ability profiles measured by CAT4.
For example, spatial training provides opportunity to reduce attainment gaps; those children whose profiles show low spatial abilities have the biggest room for growth in spatial thinking. Furthermore, relative to peers, children from disadvantaged backgrounds have lower spatial skills (Verdine et al., 2014), lower spatial language (Bower et al., 2020), reduced access to spatial activities and toys (Levine et al., 2012) and are at risk of experiencing lower quality parent-child interaction during spatial play (Jirout & Newcombe, 2015). In summary, spatial thinking is ripe for reducing attainment gaps and increasing school readiness.
Spatial training provides opportunity to reduce attainment gaps; those children whose profiles show low spatial abilities have the biggest room for growth in spatial thinking.
In our recent survey (Bates et al., 2022), practitioners told us that one barrier to nurturing spatial thinking in the classroom was limited training and subject knowledge. To address this, I was recently part of the Early Childhood Maths Group team that launched the Spatial Reasoning Toolkit, a resource for teachers, practitioners and families to support spatial thinking of children between the ages of birth and seven years.
The toolkit includes a developmental trajectory and guidance document, four pull-out posters, five videos and a spatial thinking book list. Below I provide some explicit examples of how to spatialise the curriculum, many drawn from our toolkit. Practitioners can use these flexibly within their own professional judgement, to nurture spatial thinking.
Emily Farran is a Professor of Cognitive Development and Director of the Cognition, Genes and Developmental Variability (CoGDeV) lab in the School of Psychology, University of Surrey, UK. Her research centres on spatial cognition in typical and atypical development. Her research with early years and school age children relates to the relationship between spatial abilities and mathematics and science. She was recently part of the team that launched the Spatial Reasoning toolkit, a collaboration between the Early Childhood Mathematics Group and the University of Surrey.
If you would like to speak to one of our specialists about how we can support your students taking the big step into a new school, add your details to our form and we’ll get in touch
Quickly securing a sound transition into secondary school is vital in ensuring students go on to achieve their potential at school, in GCSEs and beyond. Find out how our assessments can help.